Skip to main content

A relationship between oxytocin and anxiety of romantic attachment

Abstract

The formation of social bonding is fundamental for several animals, including humans, for its relevant and obvious impact upon reproduction and, thus, survival of the species. Recent data would suggest that oxytocin might be one of the mediators of this process. Given the paucity of data on the possible involvement of oxytocin in human attachment, the present study was aimed to explore the possible relationships between the plasma levels of this neuropeptide and romantic attachment in healthy subjects. Forty-five healthy subjects who volunteered for the study, were included in the study. The romantic attachment was assessed using the Italian version of the so-called "Experiences in Close Relationships" (ECR), a self-report questionnaire for measuring this parameter in adults. The results showed that attachment anxiety and oxytocin are positively linked in romantic attachment to a statistically significant degree (r = 0.30, p = 0.04), that is, the higher the oxytocin levels the higher the score on the anxiety scale of the ECR. The authors suggest the hypothesis that this link represents one of the biological processes resulting in those rewarding emotions related to romantic attachment.

Background

Humans are obliged to face a paradox which is fundamental to the survival of the species: they are attracted to, courted by and breed with genetically unrelated individuals whom they would otherwise instinctively avoid. Romantic attachment is the psychological strategy which enables us to overcome neophobia and to mate with and create a strong, often life-long bond with a complete stranger, so that we may produce healthier offspring. On the same time, we are rewarded by a deep sense of pleasure and satisfaction through the intervention of specific neural substrates which have become the topic of recent investigations [1]. Besides neural networks, within the last decade, various sets of data have highlighted the possible role of oxytocin and vasopressin, two small neuropeptides composed by 9 aminoacids and synthesized in the paraventricular and in the supraoptic nuclei of the hypothalamus [2], in linking social signals with cognition, behaviours and reward [3]. Oxytocin and vasopressin, in fact, have been shown to be involved in the creation of pair-bonding in monogamous rodents, such as prairie voles [4], in maternal behaviour in rats [5–7], in the postpartum acceptance of offspring in sheep [8, 9], and the relief of distress vocalization in rat pups [10].

The relevance of these intriguing findings for humans has not been clarified as yet [11, 12]. Oxytocin receptors in the human brain are mainly distributed in the substantia nigra, globus pallidus, anterior cingulate and medial insula, areas which have been shown to be activated in adults while looking at pictures of their partners, or in mothers while looking at their children [1, 13, 14] and belong to the recently-hypothesized circuits of the "social brain" [15–17]. It is interesting to note that oxytocin seems to be released during sexual intercourse and orgasm [18] and during the application of different relaxation techniques [19, 20], so that it is thought to be one of the promoters of attachment and/or mediators in the decrease of the stress responses which are related to positive social bonding [21–23].

Although the measurement of oxytocin plasma levels represents the easiest and relatively noninvasive way of evaluating its presence in humans, it is still unclear whether plasma levels reliably reflect concentrations in the brain, since it would seem that central and peripheral oxytocin derive from different hypothalamus cell populations [4, 9]. However, different convergent observations on parallel changes in oxytocin levels in plasma and in the brain [20, 24, 25] would suggest a cooperative process of regulation and, thus, support the use of the peripheral marker as a mirror of the central one [8, 26]. Therefore, given the lack of direct data in humans, in the present study we explored the possible relationships between plasma oxytocin levels and a typical social bonding of our species, that is, romantic attachment, selected from amongst the different types of attachment, because it was considered particularly relevant for adults that constituted our subject population [27]. In addition, to achieve this goal, we had to set up a sensitive assay to measure oxytocin in plasma of healthy subjects, since the available kits did not permit this assessment; the hypothesis was that oxytocin might influence some features or styles of romantic attachment.

Materials and methods

Subjects

Forty-five healthy subjects (12 men, 33 women, mean age 31.5 ± 6.2 years) all of whom volunteered for the study, were included. They were residents, post-doctoral fellows or clinicians at the Specialty School of Psychiatry, or students at the Faculty of Medicine and Surgery, all at the University of Pisa. In terms of marital status, 32 (71.1 %) individuals were single, 12 (26.7 %) were married and 1 (2.2 %) was divorced.

No subject had a family or personal history of any major psychiatric disorder, or had ever taken regularly psychotropic drugs, as assessed by a detailed psychiatric interview conducted by one of the authors (DM). All subjects were free of physical illness, were neither heavy cigarette smokers, nor belonged to groups of high-risk HIV individuals, nor did take any regular medication or drugs of addiction. The women had normal menstrual cycles and did not take contraceptive pills; their blood was drawn in the early follicular phase (between the second and the fifth day of the menses). The men had no history of genital disease or hypogonadism. All these information derived from the medical history collected by two authors (FM, MC).

Thirty-three subjects had a current romantic relationship with a mean duration of 80.5 months (ranging from a minimum of one month to a maximum of 25 years); the remaining 12 had no current relationship.

Prior to enrolment, participating subjects gave their written informed consent to the study which was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Pisa.

Instruments

The romantic attachment was assessed using the Italian version of the so-called "Experiences in Close Relationships" (ECR) [28], a self-report questionnaire for measuring this parameter in adults. The Italian version has proven to have good psychometric properties of validity and reliability [29]. It consists of 36 items, scored on a seven-point Likert scale, with 1 indicating "completely false" and 7 indicating "completely true". In addition, it provides two scale scores measuring anxiety and avoidance, the two main dimensions underlying adult attachment styles. According to the scores on these scales, subjects can be classified in terms of 4 mutually exclusive categories of attachment. The age- and sex-stratified norms obtained in the validation study of the Italian version were used. Scores within one standard deviation from the norm were considered as normal. Participants scoring above normal on the anxiety scale were classified as preoccupied, those scoring above normal on the avoidance scale were classified as dismissing, those scoring above normal on both scales were classified as fearful/avoidant, while all the remaining participants were classified as secure.

Methods

In the months of January and February, and between 8 and 9 am, blood (20 cc) was drawn three times from fasting subjects who were sitting and relaxing in the same room at a constant temperature. Blood was collected in vacutainers containing EDTA as anticoagulant, transferred to centrifuge tubes containing aprotinin (Sigma, Milan, Italy) (0.6 TIU/ml of blood) and gently mixed several times to inhibit the activity of proteinases. Blood was then centrifuged at 1,600 × g for 15 minutes at 4°C and the ensuing plasma was collected and kept at -70°C until the assay.

Four subjects (2 M, 2 F) underwent repetitive blood samplings (n. 18) on the same day, under the same experimental conditions in order to ascertain the possible presence of a circadian rhythm with oxytocin which, however, would seem to be absent (data not shown), as already reported [30, 31].

Extraction of peptides from plasma

On the day of the assay, 6 ml of each sample of plasma was acidified with 6 ml of buffer A (1% trifluoroacetic acid in H2O) and centrifuged at 17,000 × g for 20 minutes at 4°C; after this centrifugation, the supernatant was collected. C-18 sep-columns were equilibrated by washing them with 1 ml of buffer B (60% acetonitrile in 1% trifluoroacetic acid) followed by buffer A (3 ml, 3 times). Acidified plasma solution was loaded into the pre-treated C-18 Sep-column; the column was washed slowly with buffer A (3 ml, twice) and the washing liquid was discarded. Oxytocin was then eluted with buffer B (3 ml, once) and collected into a polystyrene tube. The eluate was evaporated in a centrifugal concentrator (Speedvac) and the remaining sample was lyophilized by freeze dryer.

Oxytocin radioimmunoassay

Radioimmunoassay was performed by using Phoenix Pharmaceuticals Oxytocin RIA kit (Belmont, California, USA), with a method developed by us. The cross-reactivity of the oxytocin antibody was 100% with oxytocin and O with Lys-vasopressin, Arg-vasopressin, GH, alpha-ANP, Met-Enkephalin, GRF, somatostatin, TRH, VIP, Pacap 27-NH2. The sensitivity of the assay, measured as IC50, was 10–30 pg/tube. The intrassay and interassay values were 9% and 11%, respectively.

Lyophilized samples and standard oxytocin were re-hydrated with RIA buffer, and dilutions of standard oxytocin were prepared (from 1 to 128 pg/tube). Primary rabbit anti-oxytocin antibodies were added to each sample and each standard, except for the non-specific binding tubes, and then the mixtures were stored for 24 hours at 4°C. 125I-Oxytocin was added to mixtures which were subsequently stored for 24 hours at 4°C. Goat anti-rabbit IgG serum and normal rabbit serum were added to each tube; subsequently, tubes were centrifuged at 1700 × g for 20 minutes at 4°C. All the supernatant was carefully aspirated and pellets were counted by a gamma-counter (Wizard, Perkin Elmer, Milan, Italy).

All samples were assayed in duplicate.

Standard curve and calculations of unknown samples were performed by using Graphpad Prism3 software via personal computer programmes.

Statistical analyses

Correlations between the levels of oxytocin and the duration, presence/absence of a romantic relationship, the anxiety or avoidance scales of the ECR and the styles of attachment, as well as the demographic characteristics of the subjects, were examined using Pearson's correlation coefficient.

Oxytocin levels were compared between genders and between subjects with and without a romantic relationship, using the t-test (two-tailed, unpaired), and between subjects with different attachment styles with ANOVA.

All analyses were carried out using SPSS, version 12.01 (2003).

Results

The oxytocin plasma levels, representing the mean+SD of three evaluations performed within one hour, ranged between 0.13 ± 0.02 and 4.59 ± 0.01 pg/ml (mean ± SD: 1.53 ± 1.18), and the frequency distribution of maximum oxytocin levels was skewed to the right. No difference was detected between women and men, or between subjects with and without a romantic relationship.

Plasma oxytocin levels were unrelated with age, gender, marital status, or length of the relationship, while, as shown in figure 1, a significant and positive correlation was observed between the anxiety scale of the ECR and oxytocin levels (r = 0.30, p = 0.04); on the contrary, no relationship with the avoidance scale was detected.

Figure 1
figure 1

The relationship between oxytocin palsma levels and the anxiety scale total score of the ECR.

The distribution of attachment styles was typical of healthy control subjects. Twenty-six (57.8 %) subjects showed a secure attachment, 12 (26.7 %) a preoccupied, 5 (11.1) a fearful/avoidant and 2 (4.4 %) a dismissing style. A trend towards higher peptide concentrations was detected in those subjects with the preoccupied style of attachment, although it did not reach the statistical significance.

Discussion

The major finding of the present study was the detection of a statistically significant and positive correlation between oxytocin plasma levels and the anxiety scale of the ECR, a self-report questionnaire measuring adult romantic attachment, which showed that the higher the oxytocin levels, the higher the score on the anxiety scale of the ECR.

Some caution is, however, warranted in the interpretation of these results, given the controversies which exist regarding the reliability of plasma oxytocin levels as a peripheral mirror of central concentrations. In any case, it is worth noting that various physiological conditions, in particular labour, lactation and sexual activity, have been shown to provoke a parallel release of central and peripheral oxytocin [8, 9], that emotional states may sometimes modify plasma oxytocin [20, 25, 32], and that even the peripheral administration of different peptides, including cholecystokinin and interleukin 6, may trigger their central release [26].

In addition, oxytocin shows a pulsatile secretion, so that, in order to overcome this possible bias, all subjects included in our study underwent three blood samplings within one hour and the analyses were performed by using the means of the different measurements.

Another bias may be related to the subjects include in the study, all belonging to the medical class, so that our sample may not be representative of the general population,

In spite of the above-mentioned limitations, our findings may constitute undoubtedly the first report of a link between oxytocin and that state of anxiety which is associated with romantic attachment in our species. However, it is not possible to conclude from our data whether the oxytocin levels are in fact a consequence or a cause of the anxiety measured by the scale of the ECR, although, in line with the majority of available findings showing in animals anxiolytic-like properties for oxytocin [33–35], we would suggest tentatively that the former might be the case, and that oxytocin might serve to help to counteract anxiety – or, at least, that form of anxious stress associated with romantic attachment and deep concern over its continuance [11, 19, 21, 36].

Previoulsy, the relationship between oxytocin and human anxiety was sustained only by the indirect evidence that basal oxytocin levels correlate with measures of anxiety, aggression, guilt and suspicion [37], and noise stress provoke the release of the neuropeptide in highly emotional women [38]. Also, recent findings have reported that low plasma oxytocin levels would seem to be typical of individuals with low anxiety traits [39].

That oxytocin and anxiety may be linked in some way in the modulation of social bonding is supported also by scattered data showing that a moderate level of stress seems to promote pair bonding in different species, including human beings [40]. Pursuing this line of thought, romantic relationships, and perhaps social relationships in general, could be interpreted as amounting to stress conditions, both acute and chronic depending on the phase [41]. The role of oxytocin would seem generally to be that of keeping anxiety levels under control to a point where they are no longer harmful (in fact, low oxytocin concentrations have been linked with pain syndromes, such as fibromyalgia [42] or abdominal pain [43], but may nevertheless lead to such strategies and behaviours as are best suited to ensure a partner's continued proximity both during the first stages of the romance and subsequently. Oxytocin might thus be considered an essential element in securing the rewarding effects of a romantic relationship, as a result of its increasing a prospective sexual partner's willingness to accept the risk deriving from social contacts [23], through the modulation of anxiety mechanisms perhaps at the level of amygdala [33].

Of course, with particularly vulnerable individuals, if excessively affected by the relationship itself or by other events, these delicate mechanisms might be maladaptive, in the sense that such subjects might become too anxious and thus cross the line between normal and pathological states, even to the point of developing a full-blown psychiatric disorder [44]. Such a hypothesis might be relevant to the onset of anxiety disorders and disturbances of social communications, such as autism.

References

  1. Aron A, Fisher H, Mashek DJ, Strong G, Li H, Brown LL: Reward, motivation, and emotion systems associated with early-stage intense romantic love. J Neurophysiol. 2005, 94 (1): 327-37. 10.1152/jn.00838.2004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Buijs R, DeVries GJ, Van Leeuwen RW, Swaab DF: Vasopressin and oxytocin: distribution and putative function in the brain. Prog Brain Res. 1983, 60: 115-122.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Insel TR, Fernald RD: How the brain processes social information: searching for the social brain. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2004, 27: 697-722. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144148.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Insel TR, Shapiro LE: Oxytocin receptor distribution reflects social organization in monogamous and polygamous voles. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1992, 89: 5981-5985. 10.1073/pnas.89.13.5981.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Pedersen CA, Prange AJ: Induction of maternal behavior in virgin rats after intracerebroventricular administration of oxytocin. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1979, 76: 6661-6665. 10.1073/pnas.76.12.6661.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Fahrbach SE, Morrell JI, Pfaff DW: Oxytocin induction of short-latency maternal behaviors in nulliparous, estrogen-primed female rats. Horm Behav. 1984, 18: 267-286. 10.1016/0018-506X(84)90016-3.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Insel TR, Harbaugh CR: Lesions of the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus disrupt the initiation of maternal behavior. Physiol and Behav. 1989, 45: 1033-1041. 10.1016/0031-9384(89)90234-5.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Kendrick K, Keverne EB, Chapman C, Baldwin BA: Intracranial dialysis measurement of oxytocin, monoamine and uric acid release from the olfactory bulb and substantia nigra of sheep during parturition, suckling, separation from lambs and eating. Brain Res. 1988, 439 (1–2): 1-10. 10.1016/0006-8993(88)91455-2.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Keverne EB, Kendrick K: Oxytocin facilitation of maternal behaviour. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1992, 652: 83-101.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Insel TR, Winslow JT: Central administration of oxytocin modulates the infant rat's response to social isolation. Eur J Pharmacol. 1991, 203: 149-152. 10.1016/0014-2999(91)90806-2.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Insel TR, Young LJ: The neurobiology of attachment. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2001, 2: 129-136. 10.1038/35053579.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Young LJ, Wang Z: The neurobiology of pair bonding. Nat Neurosci. 2004, 7: 1048-1054. 10.1038/nn1327.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Bartels A, Zeki S: The neural basis of romantic love. Neuroreport. 2000, 11: 3829-3834.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Bartels A, Zeki S: The neural correlates of romantic and maternal love. Neuroimage. 2004, 21: 1155-1166. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.11.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Brothers L: The social brain: a project for integrating primate behaviour and neurophysiology in a new domain. Concepts in Neuroscience. 1990, 1: 27-51.

    Google Scholar 

  16. Bechara A, Damasio H, Tranel D, Damasio AR: Deciding advantageously before knowing the advantageous strategy. Science. 1997, 275: 1293-1295. 10.1126/science.275.5304.1293.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Adolphs R: The neurobiology of social cognition. Current Opinions in Neurobiology. 2001, 11: 231-239. 10.1016/S0959-4388(00)00202-6.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Carmichael MS, Humbert R, Dixen J, Palmisano G, Greenleaf W, Davidson JM: Plasma oxytocin increases in the human sexual response. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1987, 64: 27-31.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Uvnås-Moberg K: Physiological and endocrine effects of social contacts. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1997, 807: 146-163.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Turner RA, Altemus M, Enos T: Preliminary research on plasma oxytocin in normal cycling women: investigating emotion and interpersonal distress. Psychiatry. 1999, 62: 97-113.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Carter CS: Oxytocin and sexual behavior. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 1992, 16: 131-144. 10.1016/S0149-7634(05)80176-9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Uvnås-Moberg K: Oxytocin may mediate the benefit of positive social interaction and emotions. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1998, 23: 819-835. 10.1016/S0306-4530(98)00056-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Kosfeld M, Heinrichs M, Zak PJ, Fischbacher U, Fehr E: Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature. 2005, 2: 673-676. 10.1038/nature03701.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Leckman JF, Goodman WK, North W, Chappell PB, Price LH, Pauls DL, Anderson GM, Riddle MA, McSwiggan-Hardin M, McDougle CJ: Elevated cerebrospinal fluid levels of oxytocin in obsessive-compulsive disorder: comparison with Tourette's syndrome and healthy controls. Am J Psychiatry. 1994, 51: 782-792.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  25. Uvnås-Moberg K, Widstrom AM, Nissen E, Bjorvell H: Personality traits in women 4 days postpartum and their correlation with plasma levels of oxytocin and prolactin. J Psychosom Obstet Gynaecol. 1990, 11: 261-273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Bluthe RM, Michaud B, Kelley KW, Dantzer R: Vagotomy attenuates behavioral effects of interleukin-1-injected peripherally but not centrally. Neuroreport. 1992, 7: 1485-1488.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Hazan C, Shaver P: Love conceptualized as an attachment process. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1987, 52: 511-524. 10.1037/0022-3514.52.3.511.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Brennan KA, Clark CL, Shaver PR: Self-report measurement of adult attachment: an integrative overview. Attachment theory and close relationships. Edited by: Simpson JA, Rholes WS. 1998, Guilford Press, New York, 46-76.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Picardi A, Bitetti D, Puddu P: La scala "Experiences in Close Relationships", un nuovo strumento per la valutazione dell'attaccamento negli adulti: traduzione, adattamento e validazione della versione italiana. Rivista di Psichiatria. 2000, 35: 114-

    Google Scholar 

  30. Amico JA, Tenicela R, Johnston J, Robinson AG: A time-dependent peak of oxytocin exists in cerebrospinal fluid but not plasma of humans. J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1983, 57: 947-951.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Chalinor SM, Winters SJ, Amico JA: Patterns of oxytocin concentrations of healthy women and men: effect of the menstrual cycle and short-term fasting. Endocr Res. 1994, 20: 117-125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Uvnås-Moberg K, Arn I, Jonsson I, Nilsonne A: The relationship between personality traits and plasma gastrin, cholecystokinin, somatostatin, insulin and oxytocin levels in healthy women. J Psychosom Res. 1993, 37: 581-588. 10.1016/0022-3999(93)90052-H.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Bale TL, Davis AM, Auger AP, Dorsa DM, McCarthy MM: CNS region-specific oxytocin receptor expression: importance in regulation of anxiety and sex behavior. J Neuorsci. 2001, 21: 2546-2552.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Mantella RC, Vollmer RR, Li X, Amico JA: Female oxytocin deficient mice display enhancved anxiety-related behavior. Endocrinology. 2003, 144: 2291-2296. 10.1210/en.2002-0197.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Windle RJ, Gamble LçE, Kershw YM, Wood SA, Lightman SL, Ingram CD: Gonadal-sterroid modulation of stress-induced hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal activity and anxiety behavior: role of central oxytocin. Endocrinology. 2006, 147 (5): 2423-31. 10.1210/en.2005-1079.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Heinrichs M, Baumgartner T, Kirschbaum C, Ehlert U: Social support and oxytocin interact to suppress cortsiol and subjective responses to social stress. Biol Psychiatry. 2003, 54: 1389-1398. 10.1016/S0006-3223(03)00465-7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Uvnås-Moberg K, Arn I, Theorell T, Jonsson CO: Personality traits in a group of individuals with functional disorders of the gastrointestinal tract and their correlation with gastrin, cholecystokinin, somatostatin, and oxytocin levels. J Psychosom Res. 1991, 35: 515-523. 10.1016/0022-3999(91)90046-Q.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Sanders G, Freilicher J, Lightman SL: Psychological stress of exposure to uncontrollable noise increases plasma oxytocin in high emotionality women. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1991, 15: 47-58. 10.1016/0306-4530(90)90046-C.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Turner RA, Altemus A, Yip DN, Kupferman E, Fletcher D, Bostrom A, Lyons DM, Amico JA: Effects of emotions on oxytocin, prolactin and ACTH in women. Stress. 2002, 5: 269-276.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Simpson JA, Rholes WA: Stress and secure base relationships in adulthood. Advances in Personality Relationships. 1994, 5: 181-204.

    Google Scholar 

  41. Gillath O, Bunge SA, Shaver PR, Wendelken C, Mikulincer M: Attachment-style differences in the ability to suppress negative thoughts exploring the neural correlates. Neuroimage. 2005, 28 (4): 835-47. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.048.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Anderberg UM, Uvnås-Moberg K: Plasma oxytocin levels in female fibromyalgia syndrome patients. Z Rheumatol. 2000, 59: 373-379. 10.1007/s003930070045.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Alfven G: Plasma oxytocin in children with recurrent abdominal pain. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr. 2004, 38: 513-517.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Leckman JF, Mayes LC: Preoccupations and behaviors associated with romantic and parental love. Perspectives on the origin of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. 1999, 8: 635-665.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Donatella Marazziti.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Below are the links to the authors’ original submitted files for images.

Authors’ original file for figure 1

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Marazziti, D., Dell'Osso, B., Baroni, S. et al. A relationship between oxytocin and anxiety of romantic attachment. Clin Pract Epidemiol Ment Health 2, 28 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1186/1745-0179-2-28

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1745-0179-2-28

Keywords